Epigenetics - An introductory exposition
We may be on the verge of collectively knowing just how much we don't know. Biology stands on the brink of a shift in the
understanding of inheritance. The discovery of epigenetics -- hidden
influences upon the genes -- could affect every aspect of our lives.
The picture of a genetic makeup that fluctuates by the hour and minutes
seems at odds with the public perception: That genes determine
everything from our physical characteristics all the way to our
behaviour. Many scientists seem to think that our geners form an
immutable blueprint that our cells must forever follow. British research
scientists and Oxford Susan Greenfield says "the reductionist
genetic train of thought fuels the currently highly fashionable concept
of a gene for this or that"
Niles edridge in his book why we do it, says "genes have been the
dominant metaphor underlying all manner of human behaviour, from the
most basic to animalistic, like sex, up to and including such esoterica
as the practise of religion, the enjoyment of music, and the
codification of laws and moral strictures... The media are besotted with
genes... genes have for over half a century easily eclipsed the outside
natural world as the primary driving force of evolution in the minds of
evolutionary biologists."
The tools of our consciousness,
including our beliefs, thoughts, intentions and actions, often seem to
correlate much more strongly with our health, longevity, and happiness
than our genes do. Larry dossey, MD, observes in his much cited
publication health perceptions and survival: do global evaluations of
health status really predict mortality? "Several studies show that what
one thinks about ones health is one of the most accurate predictors of
longevity ever discovered". Studies show that a committed spiritual practice involving mindfulness can add many years to our lives, regardless of our
genetic mix.
As we think our thoughts and feel our feeling our
bodies change and respond with a complex array of shifts, each thought
releases a particular mixture of biochemicals in our organs and triggers
genetic changes in our cells. Psychologist Ernest Rossi explores in his
text the psychobiology of gene expression "how our subjective states of
mind, consciously motivated behaviour, and our perception of free will
can modulate gene expression to optimize health" Nobel prize winner Eric
Kandell MD believes that in future treatments "social influences will
be biologically incorporated in the altered expressions of specific
genes in specific nerve cells of specific areas of the brain"
Brain
researchers Kemperman and Gage envision a future in which the
regeneration of damaged neural networks is a cornerstone of medical
treatment, and doctors prescriptions include "modulations of
environmental or cognitive stimuli", and "alterations of physical
activity", in other words, doctors in the future will prescribe, instead
of (or in addition to) a drug, a particular therapeutic belief or
thought, a positive feeling, an affirmative social activity.
Randy Jirtle has discovered that he could make Agouti mice produce
normal healthy young, by changing the expression of their genes, and
without making any changes to the mices DNA, by feeding them methyl
groups. These molecule clusters are able to inhibit the expression of
genes, and sure enough, the methyl groups eventually worked their way
through the mothers metabolism to attatch to the Agouti genes of the
developing embryos.
In Laymans Terms
At the heart of this new field is a simple but contentious
idea -- that genes have a memory. This is not the same as 'water memory' or homeopathy; this has empirical scientific foundations. Suppose that the lives of your grandparents
-- the air they breathed, the food they ate, even the things they saw
-- can directly affect you, decades later, despite your never
experiencing these things yourself. And that what you do in your
lifetime could in turn affect your grandchildren.
The
conventional view is that DNA carries all our heritable information and
that nothing an individual does in their lifetime will be biologically
passed to their children. To many scientists, epigenetics amounts to a
heresy, calling into question the accepted view of the DNA sequence -- a
cornerstone on which modern biology sits.
Epigenetics adds a
whole new layer to genes beyond the DNA. It proposes a control system of
'switches' that turn genes on or off -- and suggests that things people
experience, like nutrition and stress, can control these switches and
cause heritable effects in humans.
In a remote town in northern
Sweden there is evidence for this radical idea. Lying in Överkalix's
parish registries of births and deaths and its detailed harvest records
is a secret that confounds traditional scientific thinking. Marcus
Pembrey, a Professor of Clinical Genetics at the Institute of Child
Health in London, in collaboration with Swedish researcher Lars Olov
Bygren, has found evidence in these records of an environmental effect
being passed down the generations. They have shown that a famine at
critical times in the lives of the grandparents can affect the life
expectancy of the grandchildren. This is the first evidence that an
environmental effect can be inherited in humans.
In other
independent groups around the world, the first hints that there is more
to inheritance than just the genes are coming to light. The mechanism by
which this extraordinary discovery can be explained is starting to be
revealed.
Professor Wolf Reik, at the Babraham Institute in
Cambridge, has spent years studying this hidden ghost world. He has
found that merely manipulating mice embryos is enough to set off
'switches' that turn genes on or off.
For mothers like
Stephanie Mullins, who had her first child by in vitro fertilisation,
this has profound implications. It means it is possible that the IVF
procedure caused her son Ciaran to be born with Beckwith-Wiedemann
Syndrome -- a rare disorder linked to abnormal gene expression. It has
been shown that babies conceived by IVF have a three- to four-fold
increased chance of developing this condition.
And Reik's work
has gone further, showing that these switches themselves can be
inherited. This means that a 'memory' of an event could be passed
through generations. A simple environmental effect could switch genes on
or off -- and this change could be inherited.
His research has
demonstrated that genes and the environment are not mutually exclusive
but are inextricably intertwined, one affecting the other.
The
idea that inheritance is not just about which genes you inherit but
whether these are switched on or off is a whole new frontier in biology.
It raises questions with huge implications, and means the search will
be on to find what sort of environmental effects can affect these
switches.
After the tragic events of September 11th 2001,
Rachel Yehuda, a psychologist at the Mount Sinai School of Medicine in
New York, studied the effects of stress on a group of women who were
inside or near the World Trade Center and were pregnant at the time.
Produced in conjunction with Jonathan Seckl, an Edinburgh doctor, her
results suggest that stress effects can pass down generations. Meanwhile
research at Washington State University points to toxic effects -- like
exposure to fungicides or pesticides -- causing biological changes in
rats that persist for at least four generations.
This work is
at the forefront of a paradigm shift in scientific thinking. It will
change the way the causes of disease are viewed, as well as the
importance of lifestyles and family relationships. What people do no
longer just affects themselves, but can determine the health of their
children and grandchildren in decades to come. "We are," as Marcus
Pembrey says, "all guardians of our genome."
The mechanics of epigenetics
For the materialistically predisposed, adding
small methyl groups to specific points of DNA is one of the main ways
of turning a gene off. This short
video and news report gives you a Laymans view understanding.
Due to the limited data available in the gene structure epigenetic changes
were needed for things such as plasmodium
falciparum genes.
Epigenetic changes are more flexible than genetic changes, and permit
rapid yet reversible adaptation, so determining which proteins can be
turned off, via release of the usual hormones/endorphins/neurochemicals
that relate to particular states of mind, is very important.
Epigenetic
changes determine which proteins are transcribed. So far theres three
systems which intertwine with each other to silence genes: histone
modifications (histone proteins that are the primary components of
chromatin responsible for forming DNA that makes up chromosomes),
RNA-associated silencing and DNA methylation.
5-methylcytosine is a is a methylated form of the DNA base cytosine that is involved in the regulation of gene transcription, old perspectives on it's role in this (that did not consider biofeedback and
psychosomatic cause of the chemicals in question at the time) while still true have been shown to not be holistic enough; it's now known to be a main epigenetic mechanism, with many
5MC patterns being inherited epigenetically.
Epigenetic
changes are an ideal target for disease control, because they are
natyurally reversable, whereas sequence mutations in DNA are not. Which
would shut a lot of the anti-genetic modification people up. But also
likely just move the issue they have to scrutinising epigenetics; unless
they actually understand the reversible ramifications it has that have
before been a matter of contention.
DNA Is Not Destiny (source)
"The new science of epigenetics rewrites the rules of disease, heredity, and identity. [.....]
It was a little eerie and a little scary to see how something as subtle
as a nutritional change in the pregnant mother rat could have such a
dramatic impact on the gene expression of the baby," Jirtle says. "The
results showed how important epigenetic changes could be."
Jirtle continues "The tip of the iceberg is genomics.... The bottom of the iceberg is epigenetics"
Dr Moshe Syf from McGill university in Montreal has studied the
relationships between rats and their offspring. Some of the mother rats
groomed and nurtured their young, and some hardly did at all. Rats that
had been groomed as infants showed marked behavioural changes as adults,
they were "Less fearful and better adjusted than the offspring of the
neglectful mothers" (Epigenetics, The Economist*) They then acted in
similar nurturing ways towards their own offspring, producing the same
epigenetic behavioural results in the next generation. This shows that
epigenetic changes, once started in one generation, can be passed on to
the following generations without changes in the gene themselves.
There were numerous chemical changes detected in the rats brains and
major differences had developed between the nurtured group and the
neglected group, especially in the area of the hippocampus involved in
stress. A gene that dampens our response to stress had a greater degree
of expression in the well nurtured rats. The brains of the nurtured rats
also showed higher levels of a chemical (acetyl groups) that
facilitates gene expression by binding the protein sheath around the
gene, making it easier for the gene to express. They also had higher
levels of an enzyme that adds acetyl groups to the protein sheath.
In the non nurtured rats the changes were quite different, they were
anxious and fearful. The same gene repressing substance that Randy
Jirtle found in her work (that I cited above), the methyl groups, were
much more prevalent in the hippocampi. It bonded to the DNA and
inhibited the expression of the gene involved in dampening stress. To
test the hypothesis that these two substances were causing epigenetic
changes in the Rats they injected the fearful rats with a substance that
raised the number of acetyls in the hippocampus. Sure enough, the
behaviour of the rats changed and they became less fearful and better
adjusted.
Determining Nature vs. Nurture
Molecular evidence is finally emerging to inform the long-standing debate
Psychologists, psychiatrists and neuroscientists have jousted for years
over how much of our behavior is driven by our genes versus the
environments in which we grow up and live. Arguments have persisted
because there has been little hard evidence to answer basic questions:
How exactly do genes and environment interact to determine whether
someone will become depressed, say, or schizophrenic? And can
environmental interventions such as drugs or psychotherapy really
alleviate disorders that are largely determined by genes?
The
article goes on to note that depressed and anti social behaviour in mice
is accompanied by methyl groups sticking to genes, and also extends
this research to humans, as the brains of schizophrenics also show
changes in the methylation of genes, or acetylization of their protein
sheaths.
Experiments have shown a striking link between our
state of mind, such as childhood stress, and later disease. ACE (adverse
child experiences) conducted detailed social, psychological and medial
examinations of some 17,000 people over a five year period. The study
showed a strong inverse link between emotional wellbeing, health and
longevity on the one hand, and early life stress on the other. People in
a dysfunctional family were five times more likely to be depressed,
three times more likely to smoke, thirty times more likely to commit
suicide, and ailments were much more common in the dysfunctional
families, increased rates of obesity, heart disease, lung disease,
diabetes, bone fractures, hypertension, and hepatitis. The genetic link
between nurturing and gene expression in children is also now being
traced; "One recent study suggests that children with a certain version
of a gene that produces an enzyme known as MAO-A (which metabolizes
neurotransmitters such as serotonin and dopamine) are significantly more
likely to become violent—but only if they were mistreated as children.
In this way, an aspect of human behavior might be a bit like the body of
the Bicyclus butterfly, driven to one form or another by genes that
switch in response to environmental cues, one genotype yielding two
different phenotypes for two different environments." (why we have misunderstood the nature nurture debate - Professor Gary Marcus)
And there are many more examples of our beliefs causing epigenetic
changes, the most researched is how our perceptions effect disease
progression. Gail Ironson, MD, has shown that there were two factors
that are interesting predictors of how fast HIV progressed in the
research subjects. The first was the view of the nature of god. Some
believed in a punishing god, while other believed in a benevolent god.
She observes that, “People who view god as judgemental god have a CD4
(T-helper) cell decline more than twice the rate of those who don’t see
god as judgemental, and their viral load increases more than three times
faster. For example a precise statement affirmed by these patients is
‘god will judge me harshly one day’ This one item is related to an
increased likelihood that patient will develop and opportunistic
infection or die. These beliefs predict disease progression even more
strongly than depression” (From: View of God is associated with disease
progression in HIV. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Society
of Behavioral Medicine, March 22–25, 2006, San Francisco, California.
Abstract published in Annals of Behavioural Medicine 2006)
“Spirituality may be viewed as another type of coping. Men and women
with HIV studied during the HAART era who endorsed more spirituality
after their HIV diagnosis had a slower decline in CD4+ cell counts and
better control of VL over 4 years (18). Fitzpatrick et al. (19) followed
901 HIV infected persons for 1 year and found that participation in
spiritual activities (e.g., prayer, meditation, affirmations,
visualizations) predicted reduced risk of dying, but only in those not
on HAART. Another HAART era study found significantly lower mortality
over 3 to 5 years for those with HIV who had a spiritual transformation
(20). Furthermore, the spiritual belief that "God is merciful" was
protective of health over time, whereas the belief that "God is
judgmental and punishing and is going to judge me harshly some day" was
associated with a faster deterioration of CD4+ cells and poorer control
of the HIV virus (21). Thus, view of God may be either helpful or
harmful, depending on the nature of that belief.”
Rethinking 'the selfish gene' as more a cultural icon
"genes have been the dominant metaphor underlying all manner of human
behaviour, from the most basic to animalistic, like sex, up to and
including such esoterica as the practise of religion, the enjoyment of
music, and the codification of laws and moral strictures... The media
are besotted with genes... genes have for over half a century easily
eclipsed the outside natural world as the primary driving force of
evolution in the minds of evolutionary biologists." (Dawson Church, the genie in your genes)
To fully
accept the arguments of Richard Dawkins (author of The Selfish Gene) and
his acolytes, one would be forced to conclude that "we do it" solely
because our genes are telling us to reproduce more genes; but genes
don't drive evolution, argues Eldredge (curator, American Museum of
Natural History), especially in social creatures such as humans. In this
popular science work, he discusses a "human triangle" of sexual,
reproductive, and economic behavior that has increasingly been guided by
culture over the past two-and-a-half million years. Furthermore,
Eldredge says, Dawkins' gene-centric view "has profoundly bad
implications for social theory and its political implementation."
Unexamined beliefs in this or that gene causing this or that condition
are part of the foundation of many scientific disciplines. Such
assumptions can be found in various publications, like this one aired on
NPS; "Scientists today announced that they have found a gene for
dislexia. Its a gene on choromozone six called DCDC2", the new york
times picked up on this and ran a story entitled "Findings support that
dislexia disorder is genetic" Other media picked up the story, and the
legend of the primacy of DNA was reinforced.
The main issue, in
the case of Dawkins selfish gene material and related theories, is that
it locates the ultimate power over our health in the untouchable realm
of molecular structure, rather than in our own conscious actions and
descisions.
Dorothy Nelkin in her much cited book entitled "The DNA mystique: The Gene as a Cultural Icon" sums up the point by stating
"In a diverse array of popular sources, the gene has become a
supergene, an almost supernatural entity that has the power to define
identity, determine human affairs, dictate human relationships, and
explain social problems. In this construct, human beings in all their
complexity are seen as products of a molecular text...the secular
equivalent of a soul—the immortal site of the true self and determiner
of fate."
Swaying the skeptics
Many people have come up with ways to test epigenetic ideas, and the general trend is supportive. Some of the strongest evidence comes from the 9/11 attacks effects on people subsequently suffering PTSD
and how this has made similar PTSD symptoms more likely in their
offspring (Transgenerational effects of posttraumatic stress disorder in babies of mothers exposed to the World Trade Center attacks during pregnancy - ref2 - ref3). Some studies done on hunger and lack of food during
historical food shortages in war times can still be evident in offspring a hundred years later in subsequent generations first became apparent in Sweden after a world war.
The remote, snow-swept expanses of northern Sweden are an unlikely
place to begin a story about cutting-edge genetic science. The kingdom's
northernmost county, Norrbotten, is nearly free of human life; an
average of just six people live in each square mile. And yet this tiny
population can reveal a lot about how genes work in our everyday lives. In
the 1980s, Dr. Lars Olov Bygren, a preventive-health specialist who is
now at the prestigious Karolinska Institute in Stockholm, began to
wonder what long-term effects the feast and famine years might have had
on children growing up in Norrbotten in the 19th century — and not just
on them but on their kids and grandkids as well. So he drew a random
sample of 99 individuals born in the Overkalix parish of Norrbotten in
1905 and used historical records to trace their parents and grandparents
back to birth. By analyzing meticulous agricultural records, Bygren and
two colleagues determined how much food had been available to the
parents and grandparents when they were young.
Around the time he started collecting the data, Bygren had become
fascinated with research showing that conditions in the womb could
affect your health not only when you were a fetus but well into
adulthood. In 1986, for example, the Lancet published the first
of two groundbreaking papers showing that if a pregnant woman ate
poorly, her child would be at significantly higher than average risk for
cardiovascular disease as an adult. Bygren wondered whether that effect
could start even before pregnancy: Could parents' experiences early in
their lives somehow change the traits they passed to their offspring?
It
was a heretical idea. After all, we have had a long-standing deal with
biology: whatever choices we make during our lives might ruin our
short-term memory or make us fat or hasten death, but they won't change
our genes — our actual DNA. Which meant that when we had kids of our
own, the genetic slate would be wiped clean.
What's more, any such effects of nurture (environment) on a
species' nature (genes) were not supposed to happen so quickly. Charles
Darwin, whose On the Origin of Species celebrated its 150th
anniversary in November, taught us that evolutionary changes take place
over many generations and through millions of years of natural
selection. But Bygren and other scientists have now amassed historical
evidence suggesting that powerful environmental conditions (near death
from starvation, for instance) can somehow leave an imprint on the
genetic material in eggs and sperm. These genetic imprints can
short-circuit evolution and pass along new traits in a single
generation.
For
instance, Bygren's research showed that in Overkalix, boys who enjoyed
those rare overabundant winters — kids who went from normal eating to
gluttony in a single season — produced sons and grandsons who lived
shorter lives. Far shorter: in the first paper Bygren wrote about
Norrbotten, which was published in 2001 in the Dutch journal Acta Biotheoretica,
he showed that the grandsons of Overkalix boys who had overeaten died
an average of six years earlier than the grandsons of those who had
endured a poor harvest. Once Bygren and his team controlled for certain
socioeconomic variations, the difference in longevity jumped to an
astonishing 32 years. Later papers using different Norrbotten cohorts
also found significant drops in life span and discovered that they
applied along the female line as well, meaning that the daughters and
granddaughters of girls who had gone from normal to gluttonous diets
also lived shorter lives. To put it simply, the data suggested that a
single winter of overeating as a youngster could initiate a biological
chain of events that would lead one's grandchildren to die decades
earlier than their peers did. How could this be possible? What does it all mean practically?
Meet the Epigenome
The answer lies beyond both nature and nurture. Bygren's data — along
with those of many other scientists working separately over the past 20
years — have given birth to the new science of epigenetics. At its
most basic, epigenetics is the study of changes in gene activity that do
not involve alterations to the genetic code but still get passed down
to at least one successive generation. These patterns of gene expression
are governed by the cellular material — the epigenome — that sits on
top of the genome, just outside it. It is these epigenetic "marks" that tell your genes to
switch on or off, to speak loudly or whisper. It is through epigenetic
marks that environmental factors like diet, stress and prenatal
nutrition can make an imprint on genes that is passed from one
generation to the next.
Epigenetics brings both good news and bad. Bad news first: there's
evidence that lifestyle choices like smoking and eating too much can
change the epigenetic marks atop your DNA in ways that cause the genes
for obesity to express themselves too strongly and the genes for
longevity to express themselves too weakly. We all know that you can
truncate your own life if you smoke or overeat, but it's becoming clear
that those same bad behaviors can also predispose your kids — before
they are even conceived — to disease and early death.
The good news: scientists are learning to manipulate epigenetic
marks in the lab, which means they are developing drugs that treat
illness simply by silencing bad genes and jump-starting good ones. In
2004 the Food and Drug Administration (FDA) approved an epigenetic drug
for the first time. Azacitidine is used to treat patients with
myelodysplastic syndromes (usually abbreviated, a bit oddly, to MDS), a
group of rare and deadly blood malignancies. The drug uses epigenetic
marks to dial down genes in blood precursor cells that have become
overexpressed. According to Celgene Corp. — the Summit, N.J., company
that makes azacitidine — people given a diagnosis of serious MDS live a
median of two years on azacitidine; those taking conventional blood
medications live just 15 months.
Since
2004, the FDA has approved three other epigenetic drugs that are
thought to work at least in part by stimulating tumor-suppressor genes
that disease has silenced. The great hope for ongoing epigenetic
research is that with the flick of a biochemical switch, we could tell
genes that play a role in many diseases — including cancer,
schizophrenia, autism, Alzheimer's, diabetes and many others — to lie
dormant. We could, at long last, have a trump card to play against
Darwin.
The funny thing is, scientists have known about epigenetic marks
since at least the 1970s. But until the late '90s, epigenetic phenomena
were regarded as a sideshow to the main event, DNA. To be sure,
epigenetic marks were always understood to be important: after all, a
cell in your brain and a cell in your kidney contain the exact same DNA,
and scientists have long known that nascent cells can differentiate
only when crucial epigenetic processes turn on or turn off the right
genes in utero.
More recently, however, researchers have begun to realize that
epigenetics could also help explain certain scientific mysteries that
traditional genetics never could: for instance, why one member of a pair
of identical twins can develop bipolar disorder or asthma even though
the other is fine. Or why autism strikes boys four times as often as
girls. Or why extreme changes in diet over a short period in Norrbotten
could lead to extreme changes in longevity. In these cases, the genes
may be the same, but their patterns of expression have clearly been
tweaked.
Biologists
offer this analogy as an explanation: if the genome is the hardware,
then the epigenome is the software. "I can load Windows, if I want, on
my Mac," says Joseph Ecker, a Salk Institute biologist and leading
epigenetic scientist. "You're going to have the same chip in there, the
same genome, but different software. And the outcome is a different cell
type."As Terence Mckenna first said "culture is your chosen operating system, not a reality"
Other recent studies have also shown the power of environment over
gene expression. For instance, fruit flies exposed to a drug called
geldanamycin show unusual outgrowths on their eyes that can last through
at least 13 generations of offspring even though no change in DNA has
occurred (and generations 2 through 13 were not directly exposed to the
drug). Similarly, according to a paper published last year in the Quarterly Review of Biology
by Eva Jablonka (an epigenetic pioneer) and Gal Raz of Tel Aviv
University, roundworms fed with a kind of bacteria can feature a small,
dumpy appearance and a switched-off green fluorescent protein; the
changes last at least 40 generations. (Jablonka and Raz's paper catalogs some 100 forms of epigenetic inheritance)
Can epigenetic changes be permanent? Possibly, but it's important
to remember that epigenetics isn't evolution. It doesn't change DNA.
Epigenetic changes represent a biological response to an environmental
stressor. That response can be inherited through many generations via
epigenetic marks, but if you remove the environmental pressure, the
epigenetic marks will eventually fade, and the DNA code will — over time
— begin to revert to its original programming. That's the current
thinking, anyway: that only natural selection causes permanent genetic change.
And yet even if epigenetic inheritance doesn't last forever, it can be hugely powerful. In February 2009, the Journal of Neuroscience
published a paper showing that even memory — a wildly complex
biological and psychological process — can be improved from one
generation to the next via epigenetics. The paper described an
experiment with mice led by Larry Feig, a Tufts University biochemist.
Feig's team exposed mice with genetic memory problems to an environment
rich with toys, exercise and extra attention. These mice showed
significant improvement in long-term potentiation (LTP), a form of
neural transmission that is key to memory formation. Surprisingly, their
offspring also showed LTP improvement, even when the offspring got no
extra attention.
All this explains why the scientific community is so nervously excited about epigenetics. In his forthcoming book The Genius in All of Us: Why Everything You've Been Told About Genetics, Talent and IQ Is Wrong,
science writer David Shenk says epigenetics is helping usher in a "new
paradigm" that "reveals how bankrupt the phrase 'nature versus nurture'
really is." He calls epigenetics "perhaps the most important discovery
in the science of heredity since the gene."
Geneticists
are quietly acknowledging that we may have too easily dismissed an
early naturalist who anticipated modern epigenetics — and whom
Darwinists have long disparaged. Jean-Baptiste Lamarck (1744-1829)
argued that evolution could occur within a generation or two. He posited
that animals acquired certain traits during their lifetimes because of
their environment and choices. The most famous Lamarckian example:
giraffes acquired their long necks because their recent ancestors had
stretched to reach high, nutrient-rich leaves.
In contrast, Darwin argued that evolution works not through the
fire of effort but through cold, impartial selection. By Darwinist
thinking, giraffes got their long necks over millennia because genes for
long necks had, very slowly, gained advantage. Darwin, who was 84 years
younger than Lamarck, was the better scientist, and he won the day.
Lamarckian evolution came to be seen as a scientific blunder. Yet
epigenetics is now forcing scientists to re-evaluate Lamarck's ideas.
Exposition indeed ... This feature's author obviously not a biologist, nor otherwise qualified.
ReplyDeleteBut as with 'Scientific' Creationism, such fine points may not be relevant to the purposes of certain 'expositions' ...
PERCEPTIONS OF EPIGENETICS (article) notes the 'glitter' of epigenetics, for sensationalizing media, horn-blaring bs: http://neuron.illinois.edu/sites/default/files/U3_L7_Supplement_PerceptionsOfEpigenetics.pdf
The author (A. Bird) notes epigenetics "is portrayed by the popular press as ... an antidote to the idea we are hard-wired by our genes... (it) has a deliciously Lamarckian flavor ..."
As Bird also notes: ".... faulty copying is compounded by current evidence that all histone modifications, as well as DNA methylation itself, can be abruptly removed during development, THEREBY PREVENTING THE PERSISTENCE OF THESE MODIFICATIONS IN A HERITABLE GENETIC SENSE" (caps added for emphasis)
from your quote " (it) has a deliciously Lamarckian flavor ..." " Indeed it does. Have you recently checked the main wikipedia page on lamarkism? Get back to me with what the closing sentence of the introduction into it is.
DeleteIn fact, i will save you the time "Despite this abandonment, interest in Lamarckism has continued (2009) as studies in the field of epigenetics have highlighted the possible inheritance of behavioral traits acquired by the previous generation.[1][2][3][4]"
You might want to check references 1, 2, 3 and 4 and get back to me Brian.
All the best.
Thanks for the link, but I can not help noticing that while you have quoted your own link you have not refuted anything mentioned on this page. Why exactly is that?
ReplyDeleteHey - looks like you posted a reply. Or attempted some such. But looking at it, all that displays is some sort of garbled gibberish. Maybe a computer glitch? Can you enhance, or perhaps boost your signal?
ReplyDeleteHow old are you Brian? Your over fifty I am guessing, right?
DeleteUntil you boost your posts signal to noise ratio I have no idea what your point is. Which is maybe why my reply came across as "gibberish" to you.
All the best.
Maybe if you produce a more exact critique that could become a possibility, and the signal to noise ratio could be increased. Thanks.
ReplyDeleteZeuzzz, the quote clearly tells you where the overselling of epigenetics falls down. I guess you don't want to believe it.
ReplyDeleteI actually read it all, Catmando. I just fail to see why I should take a single quote on face value, when I have in fact supplied voluminous evidence to the contrary above. If you click the first few links I included you will see I am not referencing marginal people, or journals, these are quotes from highly respected scientists in their respected fields. The fact that Mr akers has found a single quote from someone who seems to dismiss epigenetics as having any effect on inheritance (LOL) is a moot point compared to the evidence above.
DeleteAlso 50% of this has been copied from external sources, only the first few parts and a few later comments are my own compilation of evidence and quotes.